Looking for a good sample?

Let us find the best one for you! What is your topic?

Over 850,000 documents to help brainstorm your essay topic

Haven't found the Essay You Want?
GET YOUR CUSTOM ESSAY SAMPLE
For Only $13/page
5 views

Cognitive Psychology Essays

The Milestones of Cognitive Psychology Dahlia Hill PSY 360 Donna Glover University of Phoenix April 16, 2012 The cognitive approach to human and comparative psychology rests on two main assumptions, the first one is cognitive representations and processes that act on those representations and secondly humans can discover these representations and processes, albeit indirectly (Willingham, 2007). This approach offers a middle ground between B.
F. Skinner’s cut-and-dry input—output relations and C. L. Hull’s hypothetical constructs and intervening variables. In the first case, there is no room for intermediary cognitive processes between stimulus and response, outside the realm of simple associative learning. In the second case, there is lacking an explicit scientific means by which to observe objectively the theoretical cognitive mechanisms in question.
However, E. C. Tolman was the first psychologists to suggest the induction of the intermediary cognitive processes through the results of scientifically rigorous experimentation (Zentall, 2002). It is on this very basic proposition that theoretical cognitive processes is inferred by observing behavior, which most informs the developmental milestones in the formation of the cognitive perspective as a branch of psychology.
The development of the cognitive approach as a mainstream psychological perspective has four major milestones and these are the shortfalls of the behaviorist perspective of psychology, secondly, the ability for abstract constructs to account for the aforementioned shortfalls, thirdly the bridging of abstract constructs with observable mechanisms through the medium of artificial intelligence and neuroscience and fourth, the realization that the representations and processes of cognition is similar to the internal functioning of a computer.
The behaviorism of the early 20th century could not account for many observable behaviors documented by psychologists of the time. For instance, the associative mechanism of stimulus-response (S-R) could not explain the generative nature of human language or fixed-action patterns or critical-period learning restrictions. The early cognitive responses to these deficits in explanation centered on abstract constructs far removed from scientifically reproducible verification.
For example, Clark Hull defined the mechanism of reinforcement in terms of drive reduction, hypothesizing theoretical constructs, such as primary drives and secondary drives (Goodwin, 2005). Shortcoming of these abstract constructs was that they were not demonstrable through observable behavior but rather only speculative. It was not until the concept of artificial intelligence and the progression of cognitive neuroscience that the valley of dry bones, known at that time only as the new behaviorism, could analogize the speculative, abstract constructs with any corporeal system.
Artificial intelligence offered researchers the physical ability to reproduce the mechanisms of metaphysical thought that occur during human cognition. As well, the area of neuroscience offered a biological framework from which to understand the neurochemical activity of the brain, as it pertains to the activities of human thought. In addition, computers use representations (ones and zeros) to assimilate, store, and process information. It is plausible that the human brain could work in much the same manner.
The computer presented researchers with an endless metaphor of human thought they could study through the avenue of observable behavior. Cognitive psychologists have the tools, analogies, metaphors, and aphorisms necessary to bridge the gap between abstract constructs and observable behavior. The battle-cry of the early cognitive movement was that the S-R, input-output, reinforcement-punishment dichotomies of the behaviorist tradition did not fully explain human behavior.
However, incumbent on the insistence that the cognitive approach to psychology explains what goes on between the stimulus-response interplay is the acknowledgement that S-R interactions are relevant to the understanding of human behavior (Costall, 2004). In fact, the bulk of cognitive theoretical postulations follow the hypothetico-deductive method of Hull, depend on S-R relationships to explain observable behavior and coached them in Skinnerian, Tolmanian, and Watsonian terminology. It is this congenital influence of the behaviorist tradition is oth the greatest crux and quintessential salivation of the cognitive perspective. By using the hypothetico-deductive method of Hull cognitive psychologists can theorize unobservable abstract constructs and test their psychological efficacy by way of observable manipulations and observable behavior. It is in this way that cognitive psychology builds upon the shortcomings of the behaviorist perspective to bring the gray areas of human cognitions into the light of observable, verifiable scientific experimentation.
In conclusion, the progress of mainstream psychology from a behavior-only based approach to a cognition/behavior-verified approach took place as a result of the shortfalls of the behaviorist perspective. However, it is the behaviorist’s insistence on the logical positivist tradition of objectively observable behavior that is the salvation of the abstract theoretical constructs of the early cognitive influence. In this fashion, cognitive psychology is little more than the reluctant cousin of the behaviorist tradition, invoked only as a response to the inadequacy of observable behavior to explain human cognition.
References Costall, A. (2004). From Darwin to Watson (and cognitivism) and back again: The principle of animal-environment mutuality. Behavior and Philosophy, 32(1), 179-195. Retrieved July19, 2009, from EBSCOHost Database. Goodwin, C. J. (2005). A history of modern psychology (2nd ed. ). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Willingham, D. T. (2007). Cognition: The thinking animal. New York, NY: Pearson Prentice Hall. Zentall, T. R. (2002). A cognitive behaviorist approach to the study of animal behavior. Journal of General Psychology, 129(4), 328. Retrieved July 19, 2009, from EBSCOHost Database.

Sorry, but copying text is forbidden on this website. If you need this or any other sample register now and get a free access to all papers, carefully proofread and edited by our experts.

Sign Up Login We can't stand spam as much as you do No, thanks. I prefer suffering on my own